Show simple item record

Files in this item

Thumbnail

Item metadata

dc.contributor.authorHobaiter, Catherine
dc.contributor.authorByrne, Richard W.
dc.date.accessioned2014-02-14T14:01:00Z
dc.date.available2014-02-14T14:01:00Z
dc.date.issued2011-09
dc.identifier.citationHobaiter , C & Byrne , R W 2011 , ' The gestural repertoire of the wild chimpanzee ' , Animal Cognition , vol. 14 , no. 5 , pp. 745-767 . https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-011-0409-2en
dc.identifier.issn1435-9448
dc.identifier.otherPURE: 16942400
dc.identifier.otherPURE UUID: a8af9da5-6346-429c-bc03-2a2550b6a937
dc.identifier.otherWOS: 000294346400014
dc.identifier.otherScopus: 80052064909
dc.identifier.otherORCID: /0000-0002-3893-0524/work/46125076
dc.identifier.otherORCID: /0000-0001-9862-9373/work/60630593
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/10023/4445
dc.description.abstractGreat ape gestural communication is known to be intentional, elaborate and flexible; yet there is controversy over the best interpretation of the system and how gestures are acquired, perhaps because most studies have been made in restricted, captive settings. Here, we report the first systematic analysis of gesture in a population of wild chimpanzees. Over 266 days of observation, we recorded 4,397 cases of intentional gesture use in the Sonso community, Budongo, Uganda. We describe 66 distinct gesture types: this estimate appears close to asymptote, and the Sonso repertoire includes most gestures described informally at other sites. Differences in repertoire were noted between individuals and age classes, but in both cases, the measured repertoire size was predicted by the time subjects were observed gesturing. No idiosyncratic usages were found, i.e. no gesture type was used only by one individual. No support was found for the idea that gestures are acquired by 'ontogenetic ritualization' from originally effective actions; moreover, in detailed analyses of two gestures, action elements composing the gestures did not closely match those of the presumed original actions. Rather, chimpanzee gestures are species-typical; indeed, many are 'family-typical', because gesture types recorded in gorillas, orangutans and chimpanzee overlap extensively, with 24 gestures recorded in all three genera. Nevertheless, chimpanzee gestures are used flexibly across a range of contexts and show clear adjustment to audience (e.g. silent gestures for attentive targets, contact gestures for inattentive ones). Such highly intentional use of a species-typical repertoire raises intriguing questions for the evolution of advanced communication.
dc.format.extent23
dc.language.isoeng
dc.relation.ispartofAnimal Cognitionen
dc.rights(c) Springer-Verlag 2011. This is the author's version of this article. The final publication is available at link.springer.comen
dc.subjectCommunicationen
dc.subjectPanen
dc.subjectIntentional gestureen
dc.subjectOntogenetic ritualizationen
dc.subjectSpecies-typicalen
dc.subjectFamily-typicalen
dc.subjectMonkey alarm callsen
dc.subjectYoung chimpanzeesen
dc.subjectNonhuman primateen
dc.subjectLanguageen
dc.subjectCombinationsen
dc.subjectSemanticsen
dc.subjectBehavioren
dc.subjectGorillaen
dc.subjectQL Zoologyen
dc.subjectDASen
dc.subject.lccQLen
dc.titleThe gestural repertoire of the wild chimpanzeeen
dc.typeJournal articleen
dc.description.versionPostprinten
dc.contributor.institutionUniversity of St Andrews. School of Psychology and Neuroscienceen
dc.contributor.institutionUniversity of St Andrews. Centre for Social Learning & Cognitive Evolutionen
dc.identifier.doihttps://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-011-0409-2
dc.description.statusPeer revieweden


This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record