Show simple item record

Files in this item

Thumbnail

Item metadata

dc.contributor.authorTausch, Nicole
dc.contributor.authorBecker, Julia C.
dc.contributor.authorSpears, Russell
dc.contributor.authorChrist, Oliver
dc.contributor.authorSaab, Rim
dc.contributor.authorSingh, Purnima
dc.contributor.authorSiddiqui, Roomana N.
dc.date.accessioned2013-07-26T09:01:06Z
dc.date.available2013-07-26T09:01:06Z
dc.date.issued2011-07
dc.identifier10909524
dc.identifier1bc7dfde-7641-4afb-bdb1-5a8edf5397bf
dc.identifier000291898600010
dc.identifier79959836753
dc.identifier.citationTausch , N , Becker , J C , Spears , R , Christ , O , Saab , R , Singh , P & Siddiqui , R N 2011 , ' Explaining Radical Group Behavior : Developing Emotion and Efficacy Routes to Normative and Nonnormative Collective Action ' , Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , vol. 101 , no. 1 , pp. 129-148 . https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022728en
dc.identifier.issn0022-3514
dc.identifier.otherORCID: /0000-0002-9471-0673/work/46362125
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/10023/3880
dc.description.abstractA recent model of collective action distinguishes 2 distinct pathways: an emotional pathway whereby anger in response to injustice motivates action and an efficacy pathway where the belief that issues can be solved collectively increases the likelihood that group members take action (van Zomeren, Spears, Fischer, & Leach, 2004). Research supporting this model has, however, focused entirely on relatively normative actions such as participating in demonstrations. We argue that the relations between emotions, efficacy, and action differ for more extreme, nonnormative actions and propose (a) that nonnormative actions are often driven by a sense of low efficacy and (b) that contempt, which, unlike anger, entails psychological distancing and a lack of reconciliatory intentions, predicts nonnormative action. These ideas were tested in 3 survey studies examining student protests against tuition fees in Germany (N = 332), Indian Muslims' action support in relation to ingroup disadvantage (N = 156), and British Muslims' responses to British foreign policy (N = 466). Results were generally supportive of predictions and indicated that (a) anger was strongly related to normative action but overall unrelated or less strongly related to nonnormative action, (b) contempt was either unrelated or negatively related to normative action but significantly positively predicted nonnormative action, and (c) efficacy was positively related to normative action and negatively related to nonnormative action. The implications of these findings for understanding and dealing with extreme intergroup phenomena such as terrorism are discussed.
dc.format.extent20
dc.format.extent385169
dc.language.isoeng
dc.relation.ispartofJournal of Personality and Social Psychologyen
dc.subjectNonnormative collective actionen
dc.subjectViolenceen
dc.subjectAngeren
dc.subjectContempten
dc.subjectGroup efficacyen
dc.subjectRelative Deprivation Theoryen
dc.subjectGroup-based angeren
dc.subjectSocial identityen
dc.subjectAction tendanciesen
dc.subjectMissing dataen
dc.subjectTerrorismen
dc.subjectParticipationen
dc.subjectMobilisationen
dc.subjectStrategiesen
dc.subjectAttitudesen
dc.subjectSDG 16 - Peace, Justice and Strong Institutionsen
dc.titleExplaining Radical Group Behavior : Developing Emotion and Efficacy Routes to Normative and Nonnormative Collective Actionen
dc.typeJournal articleen
dc.contributor.institutionUniversity of St Andrews. School of Psychology and Neuroscienceen
dc.identifier.doihttps://doi.org/10.1037/a0022728
dc.description.statusPeer revieweden


This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record