Show simple item record

Files in this item

Thumbnail

Item metadata

dc.contributor.authorLameira, Adriano R.
dc.contributor.authorShumaker, Robert W.
dc.date.accessioned2019-08-27T12:30:02Z
dc.date.available2019-08-27T12:30:02Z
dc.date.issued2019-08-23
dc.identifier260876445
dc.identifier78ef659f-fb28-45a5-acc3-90a6b3dd2ed7
dc.identifier85071387015
dc.identifier000482396100003
dc.identifier.citationLameira , A R & Shumaker , R W 2019 , ' Orangutans show active voicing through a membranophone ' , Scientific Reports , vol. 9 , 12289 . https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-48760-7en
dc.identifier.issn2045-2322
dc.identifier.otherRIS: urn:8A8D730054EC2512EAEF3B1198B1CF13
dc.identifier.otherRIS: Lameira2019
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/10023/18379
dc.descriptionA.R.L. was supported by the European Union’s Horizon 2020 Research and Innovation Program under the Marie Skłodowska-Curie grant agreement no. 702137.en
dc.description.abstractActive voicing – voluntary control over vocal fold oscillation – is essential for speech. Nonhuman great apes can learn new consonant- and vowel-like calls, but active voicing by our closest relatives has historically been the hardest evidence to concede to. To resolve this controversy, a diagnostic test for active voicing is reached here through the use of a membranophone: a musical instrument where a player’s voice flares a membrane’s vibration through oscillating air pressure. We gave the opportunity to use a membranophone to six orangutans (with no effective training), three of whom produced a priori novel (species-atypical) individual-specific vocalizations. After 11 and 34 min, two subjects were successful by producing their novel vocalizations into the instrument, hence, confirming active voicing. Beyond expectation, however, within <1 hour, both subjects found opposite strategies to significantly alter their voice duration and frequency to better activate the membranophone, further demonstrating plastic voice control as a result of experience with the instrument. Results highlight how individual differences in vocal proficiency between great apes may affect performance in experimental tests. Failing to adjust a test’s difficulty level to individuals’ vocal skill may lead to false negatives, which may have largely been the case in past studies now used as “textbook fact” for great ape “missing” vocal capacities. Results qualitatively differ from small changes that can be caused in innate monkey calls by intensive months-long conditional training. Our findings verify that active voicing beyond the typical range of the species’ repertoire, which in our species underpins the acquisition of new voiced speech sounds, is not uniquely human among great apes.
dc.format.extent6
dc.format.extent1218528
dc.language.isoeng
dc.relation.ispartofScientific Reportsen
dc.subjectBF Psychologyen
dc.subjectNDASen
dc.subject.lccBFen
dc.titleOrangutans show active voicing through a membranophoneen
dc.typeJournal articleen
dc.contributor.sponsorEuropean Commissionen
dc.contributor.institutionUniversity of St Andrews. Centre for Social Learning & Cognitive Evolutionen
dc.contributor.institutionUniversity of St Andrews. School of Psychology and Neuroscienceen
dc.identifier.doi10.1038/s41598-019-48760-7
dc.description.statusPeer revieweden
dc.identifier.grantnumber702137en


This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record