Show simple item record

Files in this item

Thumbnail

Item metadata

dc.contributor.authorJersakova, Radka
dc.contributor.authorAllan, Richard
dc.contributor.authorBooth, Jonathan
dc.contributor.authorSouchay, Celine
dc.contributor.authorO'Connor, Akira Robert
dc.date.accessioned2018-08-28T23:33:42Z
dc.date.available2018-08-28T23:33:42Z
dc.date.issued2017-12
dc.identifier250755431
dc.identifierbe6b4aac-4e8c-4b67-851b-a8657e630084
dc.identifier85028467183
dc.identifier000412265500012
dc.identifier.citationJersakova , R , Allan , R , Booth , J , Souchay , C & O'Connor , A R 2017 , ' Understanding metacognitive confidence : insights from judgment-of-learning justifications ' , Journal of Memory and Language , vol. 97 , pp. 187-207 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2017.08.002en
dc.identifier.issn0749-596X
dc.identifier.otherORCID: /0000-0002-7943-5183/work/36476317
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/10023/15899
dc.descriptionThis research was supported by the Economic and Social Research Council Studentship awarded to Radka Jersakova [ES/J500215/1].en
dc.description.abstractThis study employed the delayed judgment-of-learning (JOL) paradigm to investigate the content of metacognitive judgments; after studying cue-target word-pairs, participants predicted their ability to remember targets on a future memory test (cued recognition in Experiments 1 and 2 and cued recall in Experiment 3). In Experiment 1 and the confidence JOL group of Experiment 3, participants used a commonly employed 6-point numeric confidence JOL scale (0–20–40–60–80–100%). In Experiment 2 and the binary JOL group of Experiment 3 participants first made a binary yes/no JOL prediction followed by a 3-point verbal confidence judgment (sure-maybe-guess). In all experiments, on a subset of trials, participants gave a written justification of why they gave that specific JOL response. We used natural language processing techniques (latent semantic analysis and word frequency [n-gram] analysis) to characterize the content of the written justifications and to capture what types of evidence evaluation uniquely separate one JOL response type from others. We also used a machine learning classification algorithm (support vector machine [SVM]) to quantify the extent to which any two JOL responses differed from each other. We found that: (i) participants can justify and explain their JOLs; (ii) these justifications reference cue familiarity and target accessibility and so are particularly consistent with the two-stage metacognitive model; and (iii) JOL confidence judgements do not correspond to yes/no responses in the manner typically assumed within the literature (i.e. 0–40% interpreted as no predictions).
dc.format.extent21
dc.format.extent1674445
dc.language.isoeng
dc.relation.ispartofJournal of Memory and Languageen
dc.subjectMetacognitionen
dc.subjectJudgments-of-learningen
dc.subjectEpisodic memoryen
dc.subjectConfidenceen
dc.subjectLinguisticsen
dc.subjectBF Psychologyen
dc.subjectNDASen
dc.subjectBDCen
dc.subject.lccBFen
dc.titleUnderstanding metacognitive confidence : insights from judgment-of-learning justificationsen
dc.typeJournal articleen
dc.contributor.institutionUniversity of St Andrews. School of Psychology and Neuroscienceen
dc.identifier.doihttps://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2017.08.002
dc.description.statusPeer revieweden
dc.date.embargoedUntil2018-08-29


This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record