Show simple item record

Files in this item

Thumbnail

Item metadata

dc.contributor.authorSchlenker, Philippe
dc.contributor.authorChemla, Emmanuel
dc.contributor.authorSchel, Anne M.
dc.contributor.authorFuller, James
dc.contributor.authorGautier, Jean-Pierre
dc.contributor.authorKuhn, Jeremy
dc.contributor.authorVeselinović, Dunja
dc.contributor.authorArnold, Kate
dc.contributor.authorCäsar, Cristiane
dc.contributor.authorKeenan, Sumir
dc.contributor.authorLemasson, Alban
dc.contributor.authorOuattara, Karim
dc.contributor.authorRyder, Robin
dc.contributor.authorZuberbühler, Klaus
dc.date.accessioned2017-07-05T23:33:50Z
dc.date.available2017-07-05T23:33:50Z
dc.date.issued2016-07-05
dc.identifier244761678
dc.identifier7fa32fab-6f08-4fec-bff4-0822ec8e0691
dc.identifier84978388493
dc.identifier000379522600001
dc.identifier.citationSchlenker , P , Chemla , E , Schel , A M , Fuller , J , Gautier , J-P , Kuhn , J , Veselinović , D , Arnold , K , Cäsar , C , Keenan , S , Lemasson , A , Ouattara , K , Ryder , R & Zuberbühler , K 2016 , ' Formal monkey linguistics ' , Theoretical Linguistics , vol. 42 , no. 1-2 , pp. 1-90 . https://doi.org/10.1515/tl-2016-0001en
dc.identifier.issn0301-4428
dc.identifier.otherORCID: /0000-0001-8378-088X/work/64360763
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/10023/11154
dc.descriptionZuberbühler: The research leading to these results received funding from the European Research Council under ERC grant ‘Prilang 283871’ and also from the Swiss National Science Foundation under grant ‘FN 310030_143359/1’.en
dc.description.abstractWe argue that rich data gathered in experimental primatology in the last 40 years can benefit from analytical methods used in contemporary linguistics. Focusing on the syntactic and especially semantic side, we suggest that these methods could help clarify five questions: (i) what morphology and syntax, if any, do monkey calls have? (ii) what is the 'lexical meaning' of individual calls? (iii) how are the meanings of individual calls combined? (iv) how do calls or call sequences compete with each other when several are appropriate in a given situation? (v) how did the form and meaning of calls evolve? We address these questions in five case studies pertaining to cercopithecines (Putty-nosed monkeys, Blue monkeys, and Campbell's monkeys), colobinae (Guereza monkeys and King Colobus monkeys), and New World monkeys (Titi monkeys). The morphology mostly involves simple calls, but in at least one case (Campbell's-oo) we find a root-suffix structure, possibly with a compositional semantics. The syntax is in all clear cases simple and finite-state. With respect to meaning, nearly all cases of call concatenation can be analyzed as conjunction. But a key question concerns the division of labor between semantics, pragmatics and the environmental context ('world' knowledge and context change). An apparent case of dialectal variation in the semantics (Campbell's krak) can arguably be analyzed away if one posits sufficiently powerful mechanisms of competition among calls, akin to scalar implicatures. An apparent case of non-compositionality (Putty-nosed pyow-hack sequences) can be analyzed away if one further posits a pragmatic principle of 'urgency', whereby threat-related calls must come early in sequences (another potential case of non-compositionality - Colobus snort-roar sequences - might justify assigning non-compositional meanings to complex calls, but results are tentative). Finally, rich Titi sequences in which two calls are re-arranged in complex ways so as to reflect information about both predator identity and location are argued not to involve a complex syntax/semantics interface, but rather a fine-grained interaction between simple call meanings and the environmental context. With respect to call evolution, we suggest that the remarkable preservation of call form and function over millions of years should make it possible to lay the groundwork for an evolutionary monkey linguistics, which we illustrate with cercopithecine booms, and with a comparative analysis of Blue monkey and Putty-nosed monkey repertoires. Throughout, we aim to compare possible theories rather than to fully adjudicate between them, and our claims are correspondingly modest. But we hope that our methods could lay the groundwork for a formal monkey linguistics combining data from primatology with formal techniques from linguistics (from which it does not follow that the calls under study share non-trivial properties, let alone an evolutionary history, with human language).
dc.format.extent90
dc.format.extent7344350
dc.language.isoeng
dc.relation.ispartofTheoretical Linguisticsen
dc.subjectCall evolutionen
dc.subjectEvolution of languageen
dc.subjectEvolutionary primate linguisticsen
dc.subjectPrimate linguisticsen
dc.subjectPrimate semanticsen
dc.subjectPrimate syntaxen
dc.subjectQH301 Biologyen
dc.subjectBF Psychologyen
dc.subjectLanguage and Linguisticsen
dc.subjectLinguistics and Languageen
dc.subjectNDASen
dc.subject.lccQH301en
dc.subject.lccBFen
dc.titleFormal monkey linguisticsen
dc.typeJournal articleen
dc.contributor.institutionUniversity of St Andrews. School of Psychology and Neuroscienceen
dc.contributor.institutionUniversity of St Andrews. Institute of Behavioural and Neural Sciencesen
dc.contributor.institutionUniversity of St Andrews. Centre for Social Learning & Cognitive Evolutionen
dc.identifier.doi10.1515/tl-2016-0001
dc.description.statusPeer revieweden
dc.date.embargoedUntil2017-07-05


This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record